Encuadres individuales, intensidad emocional y búsqueda de información

Un caso de México

Autores/as

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.29105/gmjmx19.37-481

Palabras clave:

emociones y encuadre, teoría del encuadre, encuadres individuales, emociones y búsqueda de información

Resumen

Este trabajo propone que tanto las cogniciones como las emociones conforman encuadres en las personas, y luego analiza sus efectos en la búsqueda de información sobre temas públicos y de política. Nuestros hallazgos indican que cuanto más fuerte es la intensidad emocional, más débil es la prevalencia de las consideraciones racionales en estos marcos. También mostramos que tal intensidad emocional no es despertada necesariamente por los temas en juego, como sugieren la mayoría de los enfoques de encuadre, sino por la fuente que los publicita. A partir de un caso en México, estos hallazgos son relevantes en un momento en que los políticos se están convirtiendo en fuentes directas de información para los ciudadanos en un contexto de comunicación pública donde la polarización política representa serios desafíos para la democracia moderna.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Biografía del autor/a

Manuel Guerrero, Universidad Iberoamericana

Manuel Alejandro Guerrero is Director of the Department of Communication at the Universidad Iberoamericana in Mexico City, member of the National System of Researchers, Vice President of the Network of UNESCO Chairs in Communication, and Academic Coordinator of the Professional Electoral Service at the National Electoral Institute in Mexico. He holds a Ph.D. in Political and Social Science from the European University Institute in Florence, Italy, and an M.Phil. in Latin American Studies from the University of Cambridge. His research has been focused on the role that the media plays in new democracies, especially on governmental transparency and political accountability, and on the way we use digital media and its effects on civic and political attitudes. On these topics he has a number of book chapters, journal articles, and several books.

Vianney Fernández, World Justice Project

Vianney Fernández is a researcher at the World Justice Project- Mexico City Office. She holds an MPP from the Center for Economic Research and Teaching (CIDE), and a B.S. in economics from the National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM). Her research focuses broadly on behavioral economics applied to criminal justice. More specifically, she is interested in designing and evaluating effective reentry policies and the reduction of recidivism. She frequently publishes in different outlets on these topics.

Citas

Aarøe, L. (2011). Investigating frame strength: The case of episodic and thematic frames. Political Communication, 28(2), 207-226. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2011.568041 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2011.568041

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12(5), 353-359. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00366 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00366

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323

Borah, P. (2011). Conceptual issues in framing theory: A systematic examination of a decade’s literature. Journal of communication, 61(2), 246-263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01539.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01539.x

Brugman, B. C., & Burgers, C. (2018). Political framing across disciplines: Evidence from 21st-century experiments. Research and politics, 5(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168018783370 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168018783370

Brewer, P. R. (2001). Value words and lizard brains: Do citizens deliberate about appeals to their core values? Political Psychology, 22(1), 45-64. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895x.00225 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00225

Cantor, J. (2002). Fright reactions to mass media. In J. Bryant, & D. Zillmann (Eds.), LEA's communication series. Media effects: Advances in theory and research (p. 287-306). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Cho, Y. (2013). How well are global citizenries informed about democracy? Ascertaining the breadth and distribution of their democratic enlightenment and its sources. Political Studies, 63(1), 240-258. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9248.12088 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9248.12088

Chong, D. (2000). Rational lives: Norms and values in politics and society. University of Chicago Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226104379.001.0001

Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review, 101(4), 637-655. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0003055407070554 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055407070554

Dahl, R. A. (1989). Democracy and its critics. Yale University Press.

Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. Harcourt Brace & Company.

D'Angelo, P. (2002). News framing as a multiparadigmatic research program: A response to Entman. Journal of Communication, 52(4), 870-888. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02578.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02578.x

De Sousa, R. (2001). Moral Emotions. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 4(2), 109-126. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011434921610 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011434921610

De Vreese, C. H. (2012). New avenues for framing research. American behavioral scientist, 56(3), 365-375. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764211426331 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764211426331

Dresser, D. (2020, June 10). La estrategia de AMLO: Dividir para vencer. The Washington Post. https://wapo.st/3y3iRkK

Druckman, J. N. (2001). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? The Journal of Politics, 63(4), 1041-1066. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-3816.00100 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-3816.00100

Druckman, J. N. (2012). The politics of motivation. Critical Review, 24(2), 199-216. https://doi.org/10.1080/08913811.2012.711022 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/08913811.2012.711022

Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4), 51-58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x

Escalante, F. (2011, February 1). El mexicano ahorita: Retrato de un liberal salvaje. Nexos. https://bit.ly/3DYzr96

Franks, D. D. (2009). The neuroscience of emotion and its relation to cognition. Neurosociology, 105-128. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5531-9_6 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5531-9_6

Gamson, W., & Modigliani, A. (1987). The Changing Culture of Affirmative Action. Research in Political Sociology, 3, 137-177.

Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1989). Media discourse and public opinion on nuclear power: A constructionist approach. American Journal of Sociology, 95(1), 1-37. https://doi.org/10.1086/229213 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1086/229213

Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Gut Feelings: The Intelligence of the Unconscious. Viking Press.

Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. Harvard University Press.

Gross, K. (2008). Framing persuasive appeals: Episodic and thematic framing, emotional response, and policy opinion. Political Psychology, 29(2), 169-192. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00622.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00622.x

Haidt, J. (2003). The Moral Emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Series in affective science. Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852-870). Oxford University Press.

Harmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., & Summerell, E. (2017). On the importance of both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion. Behavioral Sciences, 7(4), 66-82. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066

Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible: How television frames political issues. University of Chicago Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226388533.001.0001

Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99(3), 561-565. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.561 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.561

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 260-280. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x

Krauze, E. (2006, June 30). Lopez Obrador, el mesías tropical. Letras Libres. https://bit.ly/3SL121P

Kühne, R., & Schemer, C. (2015). “The emotional effects of news frames on information processing and opinion formation”, Communication Research, 42(3), 387-407. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650213514599 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650213514599

Lively, K. J., & Heise, D. R. (2014). Emotions in affect control theory. Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions, 2, 51-75. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9130-4_4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9130-4_4

Lodge, M. & Taber, C. (2000). Three Steps toward a Theory of Motivated Political Reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. McCubbins, & S. Popkin (Eds.). Elements of Reason. Cognition, Choice, and the Bounds of Rationality. Cambridge University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805813.009

Marcus, G. E. (2002). The Sentimental Citizen. Emotion in Democratic Politics. The Pennsylvania State University Press.

Miller, P. R. (2011). The Emotional Citizen: Emotion as a function of political sophistication. Political Psychology, (32), 575-600. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00824.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00824.x

Moreno, A. (2021, July 05). Aprobación de AMLO da un “pasito” atrás a 56%. El Financiero. https://bit.ly/3frYt6r

Muñiz, C. (2020). El framing como proyecto de investigación: una revisión de los conceptos, ámbitos y métodos de estudio. Profesional de la Información, 29(6), e290623. https://doi.org/10.3145/epi.2020.nov.23 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3145/epi.2020.nov.23

Nabi, R. L. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion. Communication Research, 30(2), 224-247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650202250881 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650202250881

Nabi, R. L., Finnerty, K., Domschke, T., & Hull, S. (2006). Does misery love company? Exploring the therapeutic effects of TV viewing on regretted experiences. Journal of Communication, 56(4), 689-706. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00315.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00315.x

Nass, C., & Yen, C. (2010). The man who lied to his laptop: What we can learn about ourselves from our machines. Penguin.

Neuman, W. R., Neuman, R. W., Just, M. R., & Crigler, A. N. (1992). Common knowledge: News and the construction of political meaning. University of Chicago Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226161174.001.0001

Piñeiro-Naval, V. & Mangana, R. (2019). La presencia del framing en los artículos publicados en revistas hispanoamericanas de comunicación indexadas en Scopus. Palabra clave, 22(1), 117-142. https://doi.org/10.5294/pacla.2019.22.1.6 DOI: https://doi.org/10.5294/pacla.2019.22.1.6

Prinz, J. (2004). Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford University Press.

Proceso. (2018, December 7). AMLO, entre la esperanza Y el escepticismo. https://bit.ly/3UOrPfq

Rapeli, L. (2014). The conception of citizen knowledge in democratic theory. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137322869 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137322869

Redlawsk, D. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision-making. Journal of Politics, 64(4), 1021-1044. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-2508.00161 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-2508.00161

Reese, S. D. (2007). The framing project: A bridging model for media research revisited. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 148-154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00334.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00334.x

Robles, M., & Salmón, B. (2018, May 1). El mexicano hoy. Igual de liberal, pero más salvaje. Nexos. https://bit.ly/2HHBcHO

Ross, L. (2012). Reflections on biased assimilation and belief polarization. Critical Review, 24(2), 233-245. https://doi.org/10.1080/08913811.2012.711025 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/08913811.2012.711025

Scheufele, D. A. (1999). Framing as a theory of media effects. Journal of Communication, 49(1), 103-122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02784.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02784.x

Secretariado Ejecutivo del Sistema Nacional de Seguridad Pública [SESNSP]. (2022, September 25). Incidencia delictiva. Gobierno de México. https://bit.ly/2ucvtV7

Shen, F., & Edwards, H. H. (2005). Economic individualism, humanitarianism, and welfare reform: A value-based account of framing effects. Journal of Communication, 55(4), 795-809. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03023.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03023.x

Sigelman, L. & Sigelman, C. K. (1984). Judgments of the Carter-Reagan debate: The eyes of the beholder. The Public Opinion Quarterly, (48), 624-628. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1086/268863

Zillmann, D. (2000). Mood management in the context of selective exposure theory. Annals of the International Communication Association, 23(1), 103-123. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2000.11678971 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2000.11678971

Descargas

Publicado

2022-10-19

Cómo citar

Guerrero, M., Fernández, V. ., & Castillo, A. (2022). Encuadres individuales, intensidad emocional y búsqueda de información: Un caso de México. Global Media Journal México, 19(37), 58–103. https://doi.org/10.29105/gmjmx19.37-481

Número

Sección

Reportes de Investigación